alcance de sonido clave permanencia del objeto en ciegos

Upload: pamela-diaz

Post on 14-Apr-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/27/2019 Alcance de Sonido Clave Permanencia Del Objeto en Ciegos

    1/8

    Reach on sound: A key to object permanence in visually impaired children

    Elisa Fazzi a,, Sabrina Giovanna Signorini b, Monica Bomba a, Antonella Luparia b,Jose Lanners c, Umberto Balottin d

    a Unit of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, Spedali Civili, Mother and Child Department, University of Brescia, Italyb Unit of Child Neurology and Psychiatry and Child Neuro-ophthalmology, IRCCS C. Mondino Foundation, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italyc Fondazione Robert Hollman, Cannero Riviera (VB), Italyd Unit of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, IRCCS C. Mondino Foundation, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy

    a b s t r a c ta r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:

    Received 24 September 2010

    Received in revised form 16 January 2011

    Accepted 18 January 2011

    Keywords:

    Visually impaired children

    Object permanence

    Reach on sound

    Cognitive development

    Background: The capacity to reach an object presented through sound clue indicates, in the blind child, the

    acquisition of object permanence and gives information over his/her cognitive development.

    Aim: To assess cognitive development in congenitally blind children with or without multiple disabilities.

    Study design: Cohort study.

    Subjects: Thirty-seven congenitally blind subjects (17 with associated multiple disabilities, 20 mainly blind)

    were enrolled.

    Outcome measures: We used Bigelow's protocol to evaluate reach on sound capacity over time (at 6, 12, 18,

    24, and 36 months), and a battery of clinical, neurophysiological and cognitive instruments to assess clinical

    features.

    Results: Tasks n.1 to 5 were acquired by most of the mainly blind children by 12 months of age. Task 6

    coincided with a drop in performance, and the acquisition of the subsequent tasks showed a less

    agehomogeneous pattern. In blind children with multiple disabilities, task acquisition rates were lower, with

    the curves dipping in relation to the more complex tasks.

    Conclusions: The mainly blind subjects managed to overcome Fraiberg's conceptual problem i.e., they

    acquired the ability to attribute an external object with identity and substance even when it manifested its

    presence through sound only and thus developed the ability to reach an object presented through sound.Instead, most of the blind children with multiple disabilities presented poor performances on the reach on

    sound protocol and were unable, before 36 months of age, to develop the strategies needed to resolve

    Fraiberg's conceptual problem.

    2011 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    In the construction of sensorimotor intelligence, vision is the most

    important sense [13]. Vision allows the child to develop an

    awareness of his external world: it is through vision that the child

    learns to know and recognise his mother's face and acquires object

    permanence and the earliest notions of causality.

    The sensorimotor development of the blind child [46] differs

    from the pattern that emerges in the normally-sighted child due to

    the presence, in the former, of specific neuromotor and cognitive

    difficulties [711]. In accordance with other authors, the mobility that

    normally follows each postural achievement is considerably delayed

    in blind children without multiple disabilities. A visual function deficit

    implies difficulties developing an awareness of physical causality, of

    spatial relations and, above all, of object permanence [4].

    Fraiberg [12,13] maintains that the capacity to reach an object that

    is presented through sound is, in the blind child, the behavioural

    indicator of the acquisition of object permanence. Also in the

    normally-sighted child, sound plays a developing role in the

    construction of the object; since Piaget's stage 4, sound becomes a

    more and more powerful cue in directing the child's active search for

    the object [14]. While locomotion plays a necessary facilitative role in

    the development of object permanence [15], motor achievements are

    in turn supported by the progressive construction of the object

    through sound. In a previous study we reported that the onset of self-

    initiated mobility in blind children is related to the demonstration of

    each child's ability to reach out and take an object presented by sound

    clue alone. In particular, we observed that all of the subjects of our

    group of blind children learned to walk independently only after

    gaining the ability to find their bearings when guided by a sound [9].

    Our survey provided further confirmation of the findings of Fraiberg

    Early Human Development 87 (2011) 289296

    Corresponding author. Tel.: +39 0303995724; fax: +39 0303995723.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (E. Fazzi).

    0378-3782/$ see front matter 2011 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

    doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.01.032

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Early Human Development

    j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / e a r l h u m d ev

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.01.032http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.01.032http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.01.032mailto:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.01.032http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/03783782http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/03783782http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.01.032mailto:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.01.032
  • 7/27/2019 Alcance de Sonido Clave Permanencia Del Objeto en Ciegos

    2/8

    and Bigelow who demonstrated that reach on sound is the crucial

    moment in terms of the blind child's access to the world of

    representation, as well as being an activity that indicates the child's

    readiness to achieve locomotion.

    To deepen this aspect, Freiberg affirms that the blind child must be

    able to overcome a conceptual problem in order to acquire thereach on sound capacity [13]: since, for the blind child, objects do

    not possess the sensory and perceptible qualities which make them

    recognisable to sighted children, in order to conceive of grasping anobject with a sound, he/she must manage without the merely visual

    information relating to that object and form a mental image of it

    [14,15]. From Fraiberg's descriptions [12,13], the acquisition of this

    ability emerges as a significant moment in the development of the

    blind child, being at once a condition of and a catalyst for all his

    subsequent development. This is why it is particularly important to

    promote reach on sound in theblindchild: it represents the solution

    to Fraiberg's conceptual problem, guaranteeing the attribution of

    substance even to objects that are presented exclusively through

    sound.

    The importance of reach on sound is analysed by Bigelow [14

    16], who has developed an 11-item protocol specifically designed to

    investigate the sequential development, in blind children, of the

    ability to seek and reach objects presented through sound and tactile

    clues. The progressive acquisition and mastering of these items appear

    to be related to a growing awareness of object permanence in the

    absence of sight. Furthermore, Bigelow establishes a correlation

    between the Piaget's stages in the acquisition of object permanence

    and the stages in the acquisition of reach on sound, suggesting that

    the eleventasks in the reach on sound protocol reflect theprocess of

    the acquisition of sensorimotor intelligence, and thus represent a

    measure of cognitive development. In the presence of multiple

    developmental disabilities this process of acquisition is likely to be

    altered.

    We set out to evaluate the development of the reach on sound

    function in a sample of congenitally blind children without and with

    other neuromotor and/or cognitive disabilities, paying particular

    attention to differences and/or similarities in their acquisition of this

    ability.

    2. Methods

    2.1. Sample description

    The study sample was made up of 37 congenitally blind children,

    consecutively referred to the Centre of Child Neuro-ophthalmology at

    the C. Mondino Institute of Neurology in Pavia (Italy) for diagnostic

    and therapeutic assessment. The Institutional Ethical Committee

    approved the research project. The parents of all the enrolled children

    signed an informed consent form in order to participate to the study.

    Congenital blindness was diagnosed according to the International

    Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health ICF [17];

    cerebral visual impairment was defined according to Good [18,19].

    2.2. Procedure

    Visual impairment was assessed on the basis of clinical behaviour

    (absence of any response to light or minimal perception of light, or

    visual acuity less than 0.02, as evaluated using Teller Acuity Cards

    [20]), and a neuro-ophthalmological assessment according to previ-

    ous records [21]. In addition,all thesubjectsunderwent a neurological

    examination and neurophysiological investigations (VEPs, BAEPs and

    EEG) in order to establish whether their blindness was isolated or

    associated with other neuromotor or sensory disabilities. The socio-

    demographic and clinical features of the sample are detailed in

    Table 1.

    All the 37 subjects underwent the assessment of developmental

    level through the administration of The ReynellZinkin Scales:

    Developmental Scales for Young Visually Handicapped Children

    [22]. For this study, we used the total scores and the cut-offs proposed

    by the authors to qualify normal mental development, slight and

    severe mental delay. Mental development was assessed at the follow-

    up session of 12 months of age.On the basis of the results of the clinical and instrumental

    examinations the subjects were divided into two groups (see Table 1):

    Group I of blind children (10 males and 10 females) with a normal

    mental development at the ReynellZinkin Scales and without

    associated motor handicaps or focal signs at the neurological

    examination. For brevity, we will call these subjects as mainly

    blind; they are, indeed, children in which blindness, at the

    moment of the evaluation, is the outstanding problem; the termmainly is used in order to not exclude the possibility of a future

    development of a neuropsychological problem, but this kind of

    handicap wasn't present at the evaluation time. We presume that,

    in the first years of life, prior cognitive hitches might be present

    but not yet describable.

    Group II of children (7 males and 10 females) with associatedhandicaps (16 with cerebral visual impairment and 1 with Jubert

    syndrome). There was clinical evidence of cerebral palsy with

    developmental delay in 88% of the Group II subjects and of

    developmental delay only in the remaining 12% (p=0.000; see

    Table 1).

    No child in either group showed hearing impairment at the BAEPs.

    The subjects in Group I were first assessed at a mean age of

    9.1 months (SD=5.1; range=424) and those in Group II at a mean

    age of 10.8 months (SD=8.2; range=628). They were followed up

    through longitudinal controls every three months for the period

    ranging from the 6th to the 36th month of age, with all the children in

    the two groups reaching the last follow up at 36 months. Infants were

    tested individually, in the presence of their mother, in a dedicated

    room in our Institution, by a familiar experimenter (always the same

    Table 1

    Socio-demographic and clinical features of the two groups.

    Gr ou p 1 G roup 2 p

    n= 20 n = 17

    Gender: male n(%) 10(50%) 7(41.2%) n.s.

    Gestational age: mean(S.D.) 38.9(0.8) 36.8(1.0) n.s.

    Born: n(%) n.s.

    At term 17(85%) 8(47.1%)

    Moderately preterm 2(10%) 5(29.4%)

    Severely preterm 1(5%) 4(23.5%)

    Visual damage: n(%) n.s.

    Complete bilateral blindn ess 10(50%) 9(52.9% )

    Light perception 10(50%) 8(47.1%)

    Type of visual damage: n(%)

    Disord er s of ext er na l p art s of the eye 1( 5% ) 0

    Retinal disorders

    Leber's congenital amaurosis 12(60%) 0

    Joubert syndrome 0 1(5.9%)

    Retinal haemorrhage 1(5%) 0

    Retinopathy of prematurity 1(5%) 0

    Retinal dysplasia 1(5%) 0

    Cerebral visual impairment 0 16(94.1%)

    Optic atrophy 3(15%) 0

    Norrie disease 1(5%) 0

    Cognitive level (ReynellZinkin Scales): n(%) 0.000

    Normal 20(100%) 0

    Slight mental delay 0 4(23.5%)Severe mental delay 0 13(76.5%)

    n.s. = not significant.

    S.D. = standard deviation.

    290 E. Fazzi et al. / Early Human Development 87 (2011) 289 296

  • 7/27/2019 Alcance de Sonido Clave Permanencia Del Objeto en Ciegos

    3/8

    operator in all the control-sessions). Each follow up comprised an

    interview with the parents, a neurological examination, ascertain-

    ment of the achievement, or otherwise, of the main gross motor

    milestones, a video-recording to explore the child's spontaneous

    behaviour in the presence of the mother. The latter was performed

    during a separate fifteen minute session of observation of both the

    baby and his/her mother staying together.

    2.3. Reach on sound protocol

    In this study we focused on the ability to reach objects presented

    through sound clue. This ability was evaluated through the Bigelowreach on sound protocol [16], the eleven tasks of which are

    described briefly in Table 2. The sequence of tasks set out in the

    protocol parallels the process of the acquisition of object permanence,

    each task belonging, according to Piagetian thought, to a specific stage

    in this process (Table 3).

    In accordance with Bigelow's indications [16], the procedure was

    explained to the mother, who was asked not to give any specific cues

    to the child during the tasks. Nevertheless the mother was asked to

    paraphrase the experimenter's verbalizations when appropriate or

    necessary to elicit the best possible response from the child. The

    infant was on the floor in a sitting position (with or without back

    support, accordingly with the age and the motor development of the

    subject).

    Each task was tried to be presented for a minimum of 3 trials in

    each session, with the exception of Tasks 1, 3, 5 and 7. We used

    Bigelow's criterion for mastery to consider a task successful when

    50% or more of the trials were correct at a certain follow-up time and

    also at the subsequent control. The task was judged successful if the

    infant reached directly to the toy used as the target and contacted it

    without any scanning or groping movements. Reaches directly in the

    direction of the toy or its sound which did not result in the child

    securing the toy were also judged to be successful if the reaches were

    off target by approximately 5 cm or less. Exceptions to this second

    criterion for success were when the child had continuous tactile

    contact with the toy (Task 1), there was continuous movement of acontinuously sounding toy (Task 4),or there were based on thechild's

    first response to the trial presentation. All the tasks were presented in

    the order described in Table 2 in all the follow-up sessions. The

    children's responses to the tasks were scored from videotapes of the

    sessions.

    2.4. Reach on sound tools and targets

    The stimulus toys used were the ones of the experimenter's and

    were the same for all the subjects. Moreover, we asked the mother to

    bring one or two familiar child's toys in order to optimise the

    children's response to the tasks. As indicated by Bigelow's protocol

    [16], mechanical-sounding toys (such as a buzzing robot or a music

    box), manually operated sound toys (such as a hand bell and a giraffe

    squeeze toy), and toys without sound (like a doll and a rubber ball)

    were used. The children's toys used as targets were squeeze toys,

    rattles, and stuffed sounding animals.

    The toys used in the intermittent sound task (see Task 10 and 11)

    were manually operated sound toys which were manipulated to

    produce a sound approximately every 2 s.

    A metal screen, 44 cm square cloths, and small pillows were used

    as coverings for the stimulus objects in the cover object task 6.

    2.5. Statistical analysis

    Continuous variables were expressed as mean and standard

    deviation (SD), and were compared using the parametric T-test.Categorical variables have been presented as counts and percentages

    and compared with the test. A pb0.05 was considered statistically

    significant.

    The statistical analysis was performed using the statistical package

    SPSS 13.0 (SPSS Inc. Corp., USA).

    Table 2

    Reach on sound protocol.

    Task n. Tasks' description

    1 Reaching of a non-sonorous object that touches the child's body

    ( continuous tactile stimulation in the absence of sonorous information)

    2 Reaching of a sonorous object that wasfi

    rst touched and then taken away while continuing to make a sound( tactile stimulation initially, followed by continuous sonorous stimulation)

    3 Reaching of a sonorous object in a fixed position

    ( continuous auditory stimulation in the absence of tactile information)

    4 Reaching of a moving sonorous object moved in a horizontal 180 arc around the head

    ( continuous auditory stimulation in the absence of tactile information)

    5 Reaching of a s onorous o bject and of a no n-so norous object that the child has dropped (obse rva tion al test)

    6 Reaching of a sonorous object concealed by a screen

    7 Reaching of a sonorous object that designs a vertical arc

    ( continuous auditory stimulation in the absence of tactile information)

    8 Reaching of a n on-sonorous object taken out of the child's hands an d positi oned in a s et poin t close to hi m

    ( discontinuous tactile stimulation in the absence of sonorous clues)

    9 R eac hing of a sonor ous obj ec t p ulled aw ay f rom t he c hild a nd m oved in a horizont al a rc to one sid e while c ontinu ing to sou nd

    ( continuous auditory stimulation)

    10 Reaching of an intermittently sounding object that designs a horizontal arc

    ( discontinuous auditory stimulation in the absence of tactile information)

    11 Reaching of an object that sounds intermittently at fixed points in space

    ( discontinuous auditory stimulation in the absence of tactile information)

    Table 3

    Correlation between the 11 tasks in the reach on sound protocol and the stages in the

    development of object permanence according to Piaget [16].

    Reach on sound

    task n.

    Piaget stages Age of

    acquisition

    (months)

    1 III: Start of object permanence 4/58/9

    From 2 to 7 IV: Active searching for the object that has

    disappeared but without taking into account its

    series of visible movements

    8/911

    8 Transitional stage

    From 9 to 10 V:The child nowtakesinto account the seriesof

    visible movements of the object, but not yet its

    invisible movements

    11/1218

    Completed protocol VI: The child has acquired the concept of object

    permanence

    1824

    291E. Fazzi et al. / Early Human Development 87 (2011) 289 296

  • 7/27/2019 Alcance de Sonido Clave Permanencia Del Objeto en Ciegos

    4/8

    3. Results

    As regards the neuromotor development (Table 4), at the last

    follow-up visit, the children in Group I showed a homogeneous

    profile:all thesubjectshad acquired good head control, andthe sitting

    and standing positions. Independent walking was achieved by 19 of

    the Group I subjects (95%) by the end of the follow up, at a mean age

    of 19 months (SD=8.7), at which stage the one remaining subject

    (5%) had achieved only assisted gait. Of the 20 subjects in Group I, 9

    didn't crawl (45%). Of the 11 remaining subjects, 4 (36.4%) achievedcrawling at a normal time, 3 (27.3%) crawled at the same time as

    walking, 3 (27.3%) crawled after learning to walk independently and

    one crawled but was not walking at 36 months.

    A very different neuromotor profile, characterised by failure to

    achieve, or markedly delayed achievement of, almost all the functions,

    both postural and locomotor, emerged in the blind children with

    associated multiple disabilities: four children (23%) did not achieve

    postural control, seven (42%) acquired only control of the head (at a

    mean age of 15 months), four children (23%) achieved the sitting

    position at the mean age of 24 months, and two children (12%)

    affected by a less severe form of cerebral palsy were able to stand at

    the mean age of 26 months. Therefore, none of the Group II children

    had acquired the ability to walk by the age of 36 months, when the

    follow-up period ended.As regards the acquisition of prehension (Table 4), all the blind

    children (100%) of Group I achieved bimanual grasp and (index)fingerthumb grasp at the mean ages of 6 and18 months respectively.

    Like the acquisition of neuromotor functions, development of

    manipulatory function was also found to be deficient in the blind

    children with associatedhandicaps: prehension was absent in 65% (11

    children) of the Group II and present, although inadequate (poorly

    functional attempts that did not result in the complete grasping of the

    object) in 12% of the cases (two children). Only 4 of the Group II

    subjects(22%) acquiredbimanualgrasp, at the meanage of 24 months.

    No member of this group acquired (index) fingerthumb grasp.

    As regards mental development (Table 1), evaluated through

    administration of the ReynellZinkin Scales [22], all the subjects of the

    Group I showed normal development. In Group II, on the other hand,

    mentaldevelopment seemedto be more impaired,with 23%(4 subjects)

    presenting slight mental delay and 77% (13 subjects) severe mental

    delay.As regards the acquisition of the items in the reach on sound

    protocol, 70% (14 children) of the Group I subjects had completed the

    protocol by the age of 36 months, as opposed to just 6% (one child) of

    the Group II subjects. Fig. 1 shows the percentage of acquisition at

    36 months at each task in the two groups. At each task, the statistical

    difference between the groups is highly significant (pb0.001). In

    particular, in Group I, one child (5%) had achieved all the tasks at

    12 months, 4 children (20%) at 18 months, two children (10%) at

    24 months, and seven children (35%) at 36 months of age. A further

    15% of the Group I subjects (3 children) achieved all the items except

    task n. 6, which the only two Group II children who completed a

    reasonable proportion of the protocol (12% of Group II) also failed to

    achieve. The remaining 15% of the Group I subjects (3 children)

    achieved only the first few tasks in the protocol. These three children

    were all affected by Leber congenital amaurosis and were character-

    ised by behavioural and relational problems. Fig. 2 illustrates the

    percentages of the Group I children who, at each of the ages

    considered (6, 12, 18, 24, and 36 months), had mastered the various

    tasks. It emerges that tasks n. 1 to 5 were acquired by most of Group I

    children by the age of 12 months, whereas acquisition of the

    subsequent tasks shows a less age-homogeneous pattern, partly as a

    result of the greater difficulty of these tasks, a drop in performance

    coinciding, indeed, with task n. 6. This drop in performance was also

    seen in the Group II subjects (Fig. 3): in this group the overall trend

    was similar to that observed in Group I (illustrated in Fig. 2) but the

    task acquisition rates (percentages) were lower, with the curves

    dipping in relation to the more complex tasks. Furthermore, at the

    ages of 6 and 12 months, none of the Group II children had acquired

    task n. 4 or above. Conversely, as illustrated in Fig. 2, zero acquisitionrates were recorded in the Group I subjects only at 6 months of age

    and, even then, only in relation to the very last tasks in the protocol.

    In Group II, two children (12% of the sample) achieved the first

    three items, one child (6%) only the first task, and eleven children

    (64%) none of the tasks. Of these eleven subjects, nine (52%) showed

    reactions indicating alertness, and two (12%) no reaction.

    In this group, only one child (6%) completed the protocol and the 2

    children (12%) completed a reasonable proportion of the tasks

    Table 4

    Gross motor and fine motor abilities.

    Group I Group II

    No psychomotor acquisition 0 4 (23%)

    Head control 20 (100%) 7 (42%)

    Sitting 20 (100%) 4 (23%)

    Standing 20 (100%) 2 (12%)

    Walking supported 1 (5%) 0

    Walking independently 19 (95%) 0

    No prehension 0 11 (65%)Attempts at prehension 20 (100%) 2 (12%)

    Bimanual grasp 20 (100%) 4 (23%)

    Fingerthumb grasp 20 (100%) 0

    0%

    10%

    20%

    30%

    40%

    50%

    60%

    70%

    80%

    90%

    100%

    Task 1 Task 2 Task 3 Task 4 Task 5 Task 6 Task 7 Task 8 Task 9 Task10

    Task11

    Group 1 Group 2

    Fig. 1. Percentage of acquired tasks at 36 months in the two groups. All the differences are highly significant (p= 0.000).

    292 E. Fazzi et al. / Early Human Development 87 (2011) 289 296

  • 7/27/2019 Alcance de Sonido Clave Permanencia Del Objeto en Ciegos

    5/8

    administered; these subjects were all characterised by cerebral visual

    impairment, cerebral palsy, and a slight mental delay. When

    observing the acquisition of the main gross motor milestones, these

    subjects were three of the less compromised in Group II. Only two

    other subjects, who didn't complete the reach on sound protocol,

    were characterised by a less profound psychomotor delay: one was

    the subject affected by Jubert syndrome and the other one had a

    severe mental delay at the ReynellZinkin Scales. Of the other 12

    children who didn't complete the reach on sound protocol, one had a

    severe psychomotor delay (he could stand seated autonomously just

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

    6 months

    18 months

    36 months0%

    10%

    20%

    30%

    40%

    50%

    60%

    70%

    80%

    90%

    100%

    TASK

    6 months 12 months 18 months 24 months 36 months

    6 months 20% 25% 15% 5% 5% 0% 5% 0% 0% 0% 0%

    12 months 85% 85% 80% 55% 50% 5% 45% 15% 25% 20% 15%

    18 months 95% 90% 85% 65% 65% 25% 75% 35% 60% 45% 30%

    24 months 100% 95% 90% 80% 70% 35% 80% 45% 60% 50% 40%

    36 months 100% 95% 95% 85% 85% 70% 85% 85% 75% 75% 75%

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

    Fig. 2. Acquisition of reach on sound function in Group I.

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

    6 months

    18 months

    36 months0%

    20%

    40%

    60%

    80%

    100%

    TASK

    6 months 12 months 18 months 24 months 36 months

    6 months 0% 6% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%

    12 months 11% 12% 11% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%

    18 months 11% 12% 11% 11% 11% 0% 11% 6% 11% 0% 0%

    24 months 22% 30% 22% 17% 22% 0% 17% 17% 17% 11% 11%

    36 months 28% 30% 22% 17% 22% 6% 17% 17% 17% 11% 11%

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

    Fig. 3. Acquisition of

    reach on sound

    function in Group II.

    293E. Fazzi et al. / Early Human Development 87 (2011) 289 296

  • 7/27/2019 Alcance de Sonido Clave Permanencia Del Objeto en Ciegos

    6/8

    for a few seconds at the age of 4 years) with a slight mental delay at

    the developmental scales, two had a less serious psychomotor delay

    (one reached the assisted gait at 20 months, one was able to stand at

    23 months) and a severe mental delay at the Reynell Zinkin Scales, 9

    had both an important psychomotor and mental delay.

    Task n. 1 was the only one that all the members of the Group I had

    completed by the age of 24 months (see Fig. 2). Moreover, children in

    Group I mastered the tasks in sequence, except for tasks 6 and 8 that

    were achieved at 36 months of age by the major part of the subjects,meanwhile the tasks n. 7, 9, and 10 were reached earlier.

    4. Discussion

    We found that the blind child's acquisition of the reach on sound

    function shows a characteristic pattern. Most of the children observed

    had completed the Bigelow protocol tasks by the age of 36 months,

    but from the age of 12 months onwards a peculiar trend seemed to

    emerge, characterised by a reduced frequency of achievement on

    tasks n. 6 and n. 8 that persisted at follow ups conducted at the ages of

    18 and 24 months, with a recovery of these performances not

    appearing until the age of 36 months. Tasks n. 6 and n. 8 seemed to

    be particularly complex for the blind child, given that they both

    present a discontinuity of the tactile stimulus. In task n. 6, the

    presence of a cover that hides the object, impeding tactile immediate

    recognition and representing a new, unknown tactile substrate that

    has to be recognised as a cover, seems to confound the subjects,

    slowing the mastering of this task. In task n. 8, the difficulty is

    represented by the presence of a discontinuous tactile stimulation in

    the absence of sonorous clues. It seems that the discontinuity of the

    tactile stimulus represents a specific difficulty for the mastering of the

    tasks by the blind children. It is only a few months after acquisition of

    the ability to walk independently through space, and thus after the

    mean age of two years, that a child learns to know his external world

    and develops the strategies needed to overcome these particular

    challenges. This would also explain whyonly a smallpercentage of the

    Group II children managed to complete the protocol, and in particular

    to acquire tasks n. 6 and 8: the presence of a motor and/or cognitive

    deficit challenges the child not so much by impeding his execution ofthe task, as by denying him the possibility of moving and gaining

    experience of his external world, and thus of finding a solution to

    Fraiberg's conceptual problem. In particular, we observed that

    walking in Group 1 begun when the first 45 tasks at the reach on

    sound protocol were achieved. This observation is in accord with

    Bigelow's observations of three blind boys [14]. Children begun to

    walk after they entered Piaget's stage IV.Independentgait seemsto be

    favoured by the appearance of the child's active searching for the

    object, activity that walking seems to support itself. This finding is in

    accordance with the hypothesis that the relationship between object

    knowledge and the advancement in locomotive skills is facilitative

    also in the blind child [14], as it is proposed to be for the sighted

    children [23]. Moreover, as observed in a recentstudy [9], congenitally

    blind children rarely crawled before walking independently and oftenmissed out this stage altogether. In this aspect, they are similar to

    shufflers or scooters who slide on their buttocks rather than crawl.

    Even though the motor responses needed in order to perform task

    n. 1 are the same as those required for task n. 3, from our results it

    would appear that blind children find task n. 3 more difficult to

    achieve than task n.1. Thus it might be deduced that a task involving a

    continuous tactile stimulus is simpler to perform than one in which it

    is the sonorous information that is continuous: in short, tactile tasks

    are easier to perform, as suggested by Bigelow's findings [16].

    The sameapplies to tasks n. 2 and 8. Again, these tasks demandthe

    same motor behaviour in order to reach the object, but the first was

    seen to be more easily achieved than the second, in which the object

    was presented only through discontinuous tactile clues: this suggests

    that sound gives to the blind child important information about the

    object, such as its location and direction, clues that a purely tactile

    stimulus that is discontinuous cannot give him. Indeed, in task n. 8,

    the child is deprived of all contact with and information about the

    object and this influences his attempts to find it. The fact that a

    substantial percentage (40%) of children were able to perform this

    task at 36 months could be an indication that the older child, despite

    being deprived of tactilesonorous information relating to the object

    when it is taken out of his hands, will still seek it, aware that it

    continues to exist even though he is no longer able to perceive it. Thechildren's performances on task n. 8, however, demonstrate that

    seeking of the target object is favoured by having previously had

    tactile contact with it.

    Task n. 4 proved to be very difficult to achieve, probably for two

    reasons: one was the lack of tactile contact with the moving object (a

    further demonstration of the importance of integration of tactile and

    auditory information in the blind child's ability to reach and take hold

    of an object), and second, the fact that the blind child, in this task, had

    to show increased upper limb control andprecision in order to reach a

    moving as opposed to a stationary object. The child's ability to locate

    an object through sound improves as demonstrated by our findings

    relating to the achievement of tasks n. 3, 4 and 7, which do not include

    tactile clues. In these three tasks, the child is required to reach a

    sonorous object that is first stationary (task n. 3) and that then moves

    along the horizontal plane (task n. 4), and subsequently designs a

    vertical arc(taskn. 7).Giventhat psychophysically it is easierto locate

    sounds in the horizontal than in the vertical plane, the sequence in

    which these three abilities are acquired reflects the psychophysical

    principles of location by sound.

    Tasks n. 5 and 9 require the child to reach a sonorous object that is

    sounding in a position different from the one in which it had been

    when he lost tactile contact with it. Children attempting these tasks

    thus find themselves with contrasting tactile and sound clues, but

    regardless of where they heard the sound coming from, our children

    nevertheless sought the object in the place where they had lost

    contact with it. Thus, even though blind children are able to respond

    to sound clues, as shown by their achievement of task n. 2, in more

    difficult tasks like these, which introduce contrasting information,

    they can be seen to fall back on old reaching strategies guided bytactile clues. Thus we can affirm that sound clues facilitate the

    reaching of the object, but only when they do not conflict with

    tactile information. Furthermore, task n. 5 was apparently easier for

    the children than task n. 9 thanks to their development and

    application of behavioural rules for the retrieving of objects they

    have allowed to fall from their hands: blind children will quickly learn

    that if they drop an object, then they themselves must go down to

    get it. Applying this rule, they do not need to postulate a spatial

    construct, which, instead, is precisely what they do when the object is

    taken from their hands as opposed to accidentally dropped (task n. 9).

    At task n. 9, children stopped and entered in a state of quiet listening

    to object movements. Their steps and gestures were guided by sound.

    Task n. 6, the one most similar to the object permanence tests

    administered to sighted children, was the one that all our blindchildren found hardest to achieve. The difficulty of this task lies in the

    need to remove a cover that conceals the object, which goes on

    sounding, from underneath the cover, for the entire duration of the

    task: achievement of this task depends on the ability to separate the

    means from the end. According to Piaget's description of the

    development of object permanence, this ability is possible at 8

    12 months (or the fourth stage of Piaget's model), despite the fact

    that, at this age, the child's perceives the object as an extension of his

    own action; in the subsequent stage (fifth stage: 1218 months), the

    child will comprehend the permanence of the object only if he is able

    to conserve a visible map of its movements. The blind child instead

    has to rely on sound clues in place of this visual information, and only

    a small proportion of our Group I subjects were able to perform the

    task at the age of 18 months. Piaget indicates that at the sixth stage

    294 E. Fazzi et al. / Early Human Development 87 (2011) 289 296

  • 7/27/2019 Alcance de Sonido Clave Permanencia Del Objeto en Ciegos

    7/8

    (1824 months) the child has acquired the concept of the object as

    permanent, although the relatively small proportion (35%) of our

    children who had achieved task n. 6 by the age of 18 24 months

    would seem to highlight the particular importance of visual

    information in the building of the concepts of object permanence

    and of the permanence of the outside world.

    Tasks n. 10 and 11, the ones associated with the fifth stage in the

    development of object permanence according to Piaget, were the

    abilitieswhoseacquisition wasmost delayed in our children.For blindchildren, objects that sound intermittently are particularly difficult to

    reach, given that, when they fall silent, their location becomes

    unpredictable; conversely, an intermittently sounding object that is in

    motion is easier to locate, because the perception of its movement

    allows the blind subject to predict or guess its position even when it

    has stopped emitting its sound (task 10).

    The difficulties encountered by the blind children of Group I in

    performing the tasks of the reach on sound protocol did not seem to

    be related to the mental development as measured by the Reynell

    Zinkin Scales: in fact, all the subjects presented normal scores. The

    three subjects of Group I with a Leber syndrome who achieved only

    the first tasks of the reach on sound protocol presented relational

    difficulties. A particular psychological fragility is described in some

    children with Leber syndrome, characterised by withdrawn and

    stereotyped behaviour, in the absence of a diagnosis of an autistic

    spectrum disorder [24].

    In Group II, most of the subjects possessed neither appropriate

    manipulatory functions, nor the ability to reach an object presented

    through sound; these children tended to use their hands as means of

    self-stimulation more than as a means through which to gain

    knowledge of the real world around them. These are behavioural

    mannerisms that have been frequently described in congenitally blind

    children with and without neurodevelopmental disabilities [24,25].

    Upon the presentation of a sound stimulus, most of the subjects

    demonstrated nothing more than aspecific reactions, indicating

    alertness, or remained immobile; at the very most, they might make

    some movement indicating excitement. The profile of acquisitions in

    this group was found to be discontinuous andlacking in homogeneity,

    and the results obtained were markedly low; however, in consideringthese low results it has to be recalled that the presence of motor

    disabilities can preclude or render extremely difficult a subject's

    attempts to reach thetarget objects. Forthe childrenin Group II, touch

    continued to be the preferred sensory channel when seeking, and

    attempting to reach,the targetobject,as demonstratedby thefact that

    tasks n. 1 and 2 (involving tactile contact with the object) were the

    only ones that a reasonable proportion of them were able to achieve;

    moreover, these same tasks were also the ones most easily performed

    by theGroupI children.The GroupII children,likethe GroupI subjects,

    showed a trend of a lower performance on task n. 6. This finding, that

    needsto beconfirmed by further studies,seemsto reveala pattern that

    could be specific to severe visual impairment, not being influenced by

    the presence or absence of an associated motor deficit.

    In conclusion, the subjects affected by an isolated visual deficit,despite showing an initial and slight neuromotor developmental

    delay, were able to develop good manipulatory skills and subse-

    quently to walk independently, thanks in part to their acquisition of

    the ability to reach an object presented through sound.

    On the basis of theresults emerging from our administration of thereach on sound protocol, we are able to affirm that when comparing

    tasks that differed only in the type of sensory information provided

    (i.e., only tactile vs only auditory clues), the tactile tasks seemed to

    be theones more easilyachieved: in particular, blind babiespresented

    an attentional alert to sound, but seemed to prefer touch to orientate

    their exploration. In fact, in the first months of life, our children

    tended to reach objects that they had previously touched rather than

    those that they had merely heard, and in the presence of conflicting

    tactile and auditory clues, they responded first to the tactile

    information, acquiring only subsequently the ability to use the

    auditory information. Only in specific situations they found to

    implement behaviours based on precise reaching modalities, like

    the ones needed to perform task n. 5. At older ages, when sensorial

    discrimination and multisensorial integration are more mature [26],

    touch and sound seemed to guide the child in reaching the object in

    like manner. The children whose blindness was associated with

    psychomotor and/or mental delay presented, in most cases, severe

    clinical pictures in which the delay, which usually persisted, alsoresulted in poor performances on the reach on sound protocol: the

    subjects in this group rarely achieved, or achieved only with a very

    marked delay, the ability to reach an object presented though sound.

    The exploratory and learning techniques adopted by these subjects

    continued to be rather primitive (the mouth playing a central role)

    and their ability to move in space was often impeded by the difficulty

    they had separating themselves from environmental points of

    reference and contact. Thus, as we demonstrated in a previous

    study [9], when the visual deficit is associated with other handicaps,

    the result is a clinical situation of extreme gravity due to the fact that

    each of the two pathological conditions seems to reinforce the other,

    giving rise to severe psychopathological pictures. On the other hand,

    the difficulties encountered in both groups (I and II) on task n. 6 (i.e.

    reaching of a sonorous object concealed by a cover) may represent a

    characteristic pattern of severe visual impairment and highlights the

    particular importance of tactile information in exploring the outside

    world and reaching the target objects.

    In view of the important role that certain acquisitions play in

    neuropsychological development, it is crucial that the blind child be

    enabled to acquire through hisothersenses theknowledge thathe is not

    permitted to acquire through sight. The acquisition ofreach on sound,

    representing the solution to Fraiberg's conceptual problem, emerges

    as a significant moment in the development of the blind child, being at

    once a condition of and a catalyst for all his subsequent development.

    References

    [1] Piaget J. The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University

    Press; 1952.[2] Piaget J. The construction of reality in the child. New York: Ballantine; 1954.[3] Piaget J. Genetic epistemology. New York: Columbia University Press; 1970.[4] Rogers SJ, Puchalski CB. Development of object permanence in visually impaired

    infants. J Vis Impair Blind 1988;82(4):13742.[5] Gillman AE. Handicap and cognition: visual deprivation and the rate of motor

    development in infants. New Outlook Blind 1973;67:303 14.[6] Langley MB. Psychoeducational assessment of the multiply handicapped blind

    child: issues and methods. Educ Vis Handicap 1979;10:97 114.[7] Fraiberg S, Freedman D. Studies of the ego development of the congenitally blind

    child. Psychoanal Study Child 1964;19:13369.[8] Fraiberg S. Parallel and divergent patterns in blind and sighted infants. Psychoanal

    Study Child 1968;23:264300.[9] Fazzi E, Lanners J, Ferrari-Ginevra O, Achille C, Luparia A, Signorini S, et al. Gross

    motor development andreach on sound ascriticaltoolsfor thedevelopmentof theblind child. Brain Dev 2002;24:26975.

    [10] Prechtl HF, Cioni G, Einspieler C, Bos AF, Ferrari F. Role of vision on early motordevelopment: lessons from the blind. Dev Med Child Neurol 2001;43:198 201.

    [11] Fazzi E. Theeffect of impaired visionon development.In: DuttonG, BaxM, editors.Visual impairment in children due to damage to the brain: clinics indevelopmental medicine. Wiley-Blackwell; 2010.

    [12] Fraiberg S,Siegel BC, Gibson R. The role of sound in the search behaviour of a blindinfant. Psychoanal Study Child 1966;21:32757.

    [13] Fraiberg S. Insights from the blind: comparative studies of blind and sightedinfants. New York: Plenum Press; 1977.

    [14] Bigelow AE. The development of the use of sound in the search behaviour ofinfants. Dev Psychol 1983;19:31721.

    [15] Bigelow AE. Locomotion and search behaviour in blind infants. Infant Behav Dev1992;15:17089.

    [16] Bigelow AE. The development of reaching in blind children. Br J Dev Psychol1986;4:35566.

    [17] World Health Organization. International Classification of Functioning, Disabilityand Health (ICF): a manual of classification relating to the consequences ofdisease. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2001.

    [18] Good WV, Jan JE, Burden SK, Skoczenski A, Candy R. Recent advances in corticalvisual impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol 2001;43:5660.

    [19] Good WV. Cortical visual impairment: new directions. Optom Vis Sci 2009;86(6):

    663

    5.

    295E. Fazzi et al. / Early Human Development 87 (2011) 289 296

  • 7/27/2019 Alcance de Sonido Clave Permanencia Del Objeto en Ciegos

    8/8

    [20] McDonald MA. Assessment of visual acuity in toddlers. Surv Ophthalmol 1986;31:189210.

    [21] Fazzi E, Signorini SG, Bova SM, La Piana R, Ondei P, Bertone C, et al. Spectrum ofvisual disorders in children with cerebral visual impairment. J Child Neurol2007;22(3):294301.

    [22] Reynell J. The ReynellZinkin scales: developmental scales for young visuallyhandicapped children. Windsor: Nfer Nelson; 1979.

    [23] Zelazo PR. Learning to walk: recognition of high order influences. In: Lipsitt L,Rovee-Collier C, editors. Advanced in infancy research, Vol. 3. Norwood, NJ: Ablex;1984.

    [24] Fazzi E, Rossi M, Signorini S, Rossi G, Bianchi PE, Lanzi G. Leber's congenitalamaurosis: is there an autistic component? Dev Med Child Neurol 2007;49(7):5037.

    [25] Fazzi E, Lanners J, Danova S, Ferrarri-Ginevra O, Gheza C, Luparia A, et al.Stereotyped behaviours in blind children. Brain Dev 1999;21(8):522 8.

    [26] Stein BE, Perrault Jr TJ, Stanford TR, Rowland BA. Postnatal experiences influencehow the brain integrates information from different senses. Front Integr Neurosci2009;3:21.

    296 E. Fazzi et al. / Early Human Development 87 (2011) 289 296